Critical Book Review
Critical
Book Review
Phonology Analysis and Theory
Edmund
Gusmann
2002
ISBN 978-0-511-07590-2
New York
The Edinburgh
Building,Cambrige CB2 2RU.United Kingdom
Published in the United
States of America
By Cambrige University
Press,New York
1.1 Syllable
Structure and Phonological Effects: Quantity in Icelandic.
In
the phonology of Modern Icelandic, namely vowel quantity. We will try to see
what the theoretical apparatus developed so far can do to cope with the facts
of the language, and also what the facts of the language can tell us about the
nature of phonological regularities. Although our main concern will be vocalic
length or quantity, we will also need to look at a few other phenomena that are
closely connected with it. This reflects the predominant situation in
phonology, where very few regularities in the language can be analysed in
complete isolation from other data. Normally, phonological regularities are
connected in various ways and can only be properly appreciated and formulated
when studied jointly. Needless to say, we cannot go into too many details here
and for this reason the regularities other than those controlling vowel
quantity will be presented briefly, and only to the extent that they are
relevant to the discussion.
a. Preliminaries
Let
us start by listing the main vowels and consonants of the language. As we
pointed out in the previous chapter, such lists are of questionable
phonological significance but they give some idea of the sounds of the
language. In Icelandic vowels are classified on the basis of the phonetic
oppositions front – back, rounded – unrounded and high – mid – low. Apart from the above monophthongs, there are
five diphthongs, namely [ei, ai,au, ou, i].
What is characteristic and striking about Icelandic diphthongs is that, just
like monophthongs, they can be either short or long. We will return to this point
presently.
b.
Open syllable lengthening
Icelandic
vowels can be long or short in a way which is reminiscent of the Italian
lengthening discussed in 5.6. According to standard textbooks stressed, i.e.
normally initial nuclei are long in Modern Icelandic in one of the following
situations:
1. they
are word-final, e.g.:
·
b´u [pu_] ‘estate’, tvo [thvɔ_] ‘two,
acc. masc.’, foe [fai_] ‘I get’
2. they
are followed by a single consonant, e.g.:
·
stara [sta_ra] ‘stare’, l´u a [lu_ea]
‘halibut’, foeri [fai_r]
‘opportunity,
·
kj¨ot [ch__th] ‘meat’, hj´on [cou_n]
‘couple’
A few initial comments are in order with respect to
this traditional formulation. For one thing, the stressed vowel referred to in
the statement does not have to be primarily stressed but may be secondarily
stressed. Further, the claim that the sequence /khj/ is one of the clusters
described in [6c] is probably unnecessary if we recognise the existence of the
palatal plosive [ch]; theword reykja [rei_cha] ‘smoke’ has a long nucleus
because it is followed by a single palatal plosive rather than a consonant
sequence. Leaving aside these additions, we can note the problems that the
listing in reveals. The obvious task is to try and reduce the three different
contexts to some kind of common denominator, apart from the requirement that the
long nucleus must be stressed. We would like to be able
to see why it is that the word-final position appears to induce the same
behaviour as the context of a following single consonant or a specific
consonant combination. Even more, we would like to enquire about the nature of
the lengthening cluster and ask what it is that causes precisely these
consonant combinations rather than any others to act as lengthening contexts.
From the above
explanation that has been explained by Edmund Gusmann is clear enough to understand by the reader.
So in this chapter I can included interested in the conditions determining the
length and shortness of stressed vowels in the present-day language. And This
chapter has been entirely devoted to an in-depth analysis of one phenomenon in
the phonology of Modern Icelandic. We have been As is frequently the case in
phonology, what starts off by being a trivial-looking minor problem develops
into an intricate regularity or set of regularities with significant
theoretical implications.In the Icelandic case we formulated a simple condition
connecting vowel length with rhyme structure: if the rhyme contains no
consonantal coda, the nucleus must branch.We found evidence supporting this
generalisation both within native vocabulary and also in loan-words, including
proper names. Once we realised that vowel length is controlled by the openness
of syllables we changed our strategy and set ourselves a new task. Assuming
that a long vowel implies that there is no following coda, and that a short one
necessarily entails one, we looked at more data in Icelandic in an attempt to
find out what the quality generalisation can reveal about the structure of
onsets, codas and coda–onset contacts.
In
this way we started by using phonological theory to help us understand a
language-specific problem, and then exploited what we hoped were reliable
results to probe further theoretical questions. Two issues emerged as
particularly relevant: the status of final consonants and of coda–onset
contacts.
Icelandic
vowel quantity provides additional support for the claim that wordfinal consonants
are onsets licensed by empty nuclei. What is singularly striking about
Icelandic is theway it reveals that final consonant sequences can be branching.
So
here I can include that English language and Iclandic language have some
similarities like from alphabet in Iclandic and English. And have pronouncation
which almost the same.
II.
The
Phonology of Polish
(second
edition)
Edmund
Gusmann
2007
ISBN 1 3 5 7 9 1 9 0 5 6 4 2
New York
The Edinburgh
Building,Cambrige CB2 2RU.United Kingdom
Published in the United
States
By Oxford University
Press,Inc.,New York
II.1. Structure
of The Syllable and The Vowel Presence
Polish
is consonantal clusters that have bedevilled phonologists of all sorts of
theoretical persuasion are studied within the highly constrained GP theory of
syllabic constituents. Existing consonant combinations result from licit
constituents such as a branching onset or a coda and a branching-onset contact,
but they also arise as a result of the vowel–zero alternations. These are
interpreted as a consequence of a morphophonological mechanism which attaches
floating melodies in some contexts but not in others. In addition to a phonetic
zero resulting from an unattached floating melody we also recognize empty
nuclei; these are subject to the condition that domain-internally no sequences
of such nuclei are tolerated. The interaction of floating melodies and empty
nuclei is analyzed on the basis of the behaviour of certain prepositions and
prefixes; morphology is claimed to play an indirect role in the pattern by
supplying or adjusting domain boundaries. The absolute majority of consonantal
clusters—initial, medial, and final—are shown to arise due to unattached
floating melodies and empty nuclei.The mechanisms controlling floating melodies
and empty nuclei are predominantly morphophonological. An additional type of
morphophonological regularity is developed which relates lexical items rather
than transforming or replacing segments in specified contexts.
The
structure of the Polish syllable seems to hold a particular fascination
forphonologists not directly concerned
with Slavic languages almost exclusively on account of the consonantal clusters
that the language allows. These can not only reach four or five elements but,
fundamentally, the arrangement or order of the consonants in a sequence appears
to defy principles established on the basis of well- (or, at least, better-)
behaved languages. There are words galore of the type łgarstw [wgarstf] ‘lie,
gen. pl.’, which are traditionally held to be monosyllabic and where the
nucleus is surrounded by consonantal sequences violating. The Icelandic
consonant system consist of 18 phonemic consonant,12 allophone consonant.
So
in Icelandic. All of Icelandician used polish language. And polish language
have phonological rule of the english. Polish syllable seems to hold a
particular fascination forphonologists not
directly concerned with Slavic languages almost exclusively on account
of the consonantal clusters that the language allows. These can not only reach
four or five elements but, fundamentally, the arrangement or order of the consonants
in a sequence appears to defy principles established on the basis of well- (or,
at least, better-) behaved languages. There are words galore of the type
łgarstw [wgarstf] ‘lie, gen. pl.’, which are traditionally held to be
monosyllabic and where the nucleus is surrounded by consonantal sequences
violating.
References
Andersen,
Henning (1969) The phonological status of the Russian ‘labial fricatives’. Journal
of Linguistics 5: 121–7.
Anderson, Stephen R. (1974) The
organization of phonology. New York: Academic Press.
Aronoff, Mark and Mary-Louise Kean (1980) (eds.) Juncture.
Saratoga, CA: Anma Libri.
Aronoff, Mark and Richard T. Oehrle (1984) (eds.) Language
sound structure: studies in
phonology
presented to Morris Halle by his teacher and students.
Cambridge, MA:
MIT
Press.
Benediktsson, Hreinn (1963) The non-uniqueness of
phonemic solutions: quantity and stress in Icelandic. Phonetica 10:
133–53.
Bethin, Christina Y. (1992) Polish syllables: the
role of prosody in phonology and morphology.Columbus, OH: Slavica.
Bickmore, Lee S. (1995) Accounting for compensatory
lengthening in the CV and moraic
frameworks.
In Durand, 119–48.
Blankenhorn, Victoria S. (1981) Pitch, quantity and
stress in Munster Irish. E´igse 18: 225–50.
Blevins, Juliette (1995) The syllable in phonological theory. In Goldsmith, 206–44.Ordab´okarsj´odur.
Bloomfield, Leonard (1933) Language. London:
Allen and Unwin.
Booij, Geert (1995) The phonology of Dutch.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Brockhaus, Wiebke (1995a) Final devoicing in the
phonology of German. T¨ubingen:Niemeyer.(1995b) Skeletal and suprasegmental
structure within Government Phonology. In Durand,180–221.
Campbell, Alistair (1959) Old English grammar.
Oxford: Clarendon.
Charette, Monik (1991) Conditions on phonological
government. Cambridge: Cambridge
University
Press.
Charette, Monik and AslıG¨oksel (1998) Licensing
constraints and vowel harmony in Turkic language. In Cyran, 65–88.
Chomsky, Noam and Morris Halle (1968) The sound
pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.
Clements, George N. and Samuel J. Keyser (1983) CV
phonology: a generative theory of the syllable. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
0 Komentar:
Posting Komentar
Berlangganan Posting Komentar [Atom]
<< Beranda